Ó£»¨ÊÓƵ

Skip to main content
  • Research
  • Published:

Assessing the impact of a mandatory calorie labelling policy in out-of-home food outlets in England on consumer behaviour: a natural experimental study

Abstract

Background

Out-of-home (OOH) food tends to be energy-dense and nutrient-poor. In response, England implemented a mandatory calorie labelling policy in the OOH sector. We evaluated changes in consumer behaviours after the policy was implemented in April 2022.

Methods

We employed a natural experimental design to assess pre-post changes in noticing and using nutrition information, and behaviours associated with menu labelling. We compared changes in England to comparator jurisdictions without similar policies. Data included four consecutive years (2019–2022) from the International Food Policy Study; participants were adults aged 18 years or older. Mixed effects logistic regression models assessed pre-post changes in binary outcomes, and mixed effects negative binomial regression assessed changes in frequency of OOH eating.

Results

In England, noticing nutrition information increased from 16.0% (95% CI 15.6 to 16.4) in 2020 to 19.7% (95% CI 19.1 to 20.2) in 2021 and to 25.8% (95% CI 25.5 to 26.1) in 2022. This increase was 4.8% points (95% CI 2.5 to 7.1) higher in England versus the comparator group. Using nutrition information increased in England from 8.0% (95% CI 7.5 to 8.4) in 2020 to 11.8% (95% CI 10.9 to 12.6) in 2021 and to 13.5% (95% CI 13.1 to 13.9) in 2022. There was a 2.7% points (95% CI 2.0 to 3.4) greater increase in England versus the comparator group from 2020 to 2021. Ordering something different was the only behaviour associated with nutrition information that increased after implementation of the policy in England: from 12.6% (95% CI 12.4 to 12.7) in 2020 to 15.2% (95% CI 14.7 to 15.6) in 2021 and to 17.7% (95% CI 17.6 to 17.8) in 2022. There was a 2.8% points (95% CI 1.8 to 3.9) greater increase in England versus the comparator group from 2021 to 2022. Frequency of OOH eating did not change after policy implementation.

Conclusions

The introduction of mandatory calorie labelling in England led to increases in self-reported noticing and using, with the key behavioural impact on ordering something different. Additional strategies may be required to maximise the public health benefits of calorie labelling.

Peer Review reports

Introduction

The out-of-home (OOH) food sector includes physical and online locations where food and beverages are sold for immediate consumption including, restaurants, cafés, pubs and bars, takeaways, fast food, street-food and other sites [1]. OOH eating has become common in many countries and is increasing globally [1, 2], typically involving energy-dense, nutrient-poor foods that contribute to elevated energy intake and increased risk of obesity [3,4,5]. There is also evidence that individuals underestimate the calorie content of foods when eating OOH [6, 7], and a recent study in England found that customers of OOH food outlets underestimated calories purchased by an average of 253 kcal [8]. In response to the public health challenge posed by the OOH food environment, some governments have adopted policies requiring mandatory calorie labels in the OOH sector to help the public make informed food choices in these settings [9].

A mandatory calorie labelling policy for the OOH food sector was signed into law in July 2021, and came into force the following year in England [10]. As of the 6th April 2022, large food businesses with more than 250 employees, including cafes, restaurants and takeaways, were required to display calorie information on menus for non-prepackaged food sold for immediate consumption [11]. Some outlets have voluntarily used calorie labelling for a number of years and a mandatory policy was first proposed in 2018 – which may have prompted others to take pre-emptive action [12]. There is evidence that some out-of-home food outlets in England began implementing calorie labelling ahead of the mandatory compliance date in April 2022. Observational research conducted between August and December 2021 found that a minority (21%) of assessed outlets had implemented some form of calorie labelling, although adherence to best practice recommendations was inconsistent and incomplete [8]. It is possible that this limited pre-implementation labelling could have influenced customer behaviour before the mandatory policy came into effect.

According to the legislation, calorie labels must display the energy content (kcal) of food for the given portion size and must be accompanied by the reference statement ‘adults need around 2000Ìýkcal a day’ [11]. Labels must be easily visible and clearly legible for both online and in-store purchases at all points of choice, defined as any place where customers choose what food to buy [11]. Exempt establishments include education institutions for pupils < 18 years, workplace canteens solely used by employees, and health and social care settings where food is solely provided for patients or residents. Specific item exemptions include menu items available for less than 30 days, beverages with greater than 1.2% alcohol content by volume, unpackaged and unprepared fruit and vegetables, and condiments added by consumers (i.e. not pre-prepared) [11]. Limited formal public health campaigns or advocacy efforts to support or explain the policy were made in the year between when the policy was announced and came into force.

The evidence for the impact of calorie labelling on consumer choices is mixed. A meta-analysis of non-experimental field data found calorie labelling interventions were associated with 21 fewer kcal selected by customers [13]. Another meta-analysis of randomised controlled trials found a reduction of 47Ìýkcal purchased after energy labelling was implemented on menus in restaurants [14]. Studies in the United States of America (USA) have found small-to-moderate decreases in energy purchased from supermarkets and fast-food restaurants [15, 16]. However, many studies in real world settings on the effects of calorie labelling policies lack a comparison group [15,16,17], and those that do are small-scale [18,19,20]. A study in Canada found that mandatory calorie information on menus was associated with greater noticing of nutrition information and that it influenced purchases [21]. Overall, there is limited evidence available for the effectiveness of national level calorie labelling policies.

There is also a need to understand the potential mechanisms through which calorie labelling policies may affect consumer choices beyond calories purchased and consumed. The present study examines other consumer behavioural outcomes to inform a better mechanistic understanding of how calorie labelling policies are associated with consumer behaviours at restaurants. A conceptual framework presented by Burton and Kees (2012) describes how calorie labels may affect behaviour. First, customers must be aware of the calorie information; second, they must be motivated to seek healthier items; third, to make the healthier selection they must have knowledge of their daily caloric requirements; and fourth, calorie labelling must provide customers with new information that translates to a different choice than they would have made without labels [22]. Consistent with this, policy impact may be limited if customers do not notice labels at sufficient rate to translate to downstream behavioural changes [23]. There are also concerns from the food industry that calorie labelling policies could potentially reduce customer patronage if it negatively affects the OOH eating experience, and these potential changes could economically harm the OOH food sector.

The present study is the first study examining pre-post changes in OOH consumer behaviour after implementation of a national calorie labelling policy in a natural experimental framework [24]. The aims of this study were to assess whether implementation of the mandatory calorie labelling policy in large OOH food outlets in England was associated with changes in: [1] noticing nutrition information [2], using nutrition information, and [3] behaviours potentially associated with using nutrition information labels. Changes in England were compared to control jurisdictions without a comparable labelling policy.

Methods

Dataset

This study utilised data from Australia, Canada, Mexico, and the United Kingdom (UK), obtained from the International Food Policy Study (IFPS), an annual multi-country repeated cross-sectional survey. Designed as a natural experimental framework, the IFPS allows for the evaluation of large-scale food policies within participating countries and facilitates comparisons with included jurisdictions that have not adopted such policies [24]. The current analysis included four consecutive years of data (2019–2022) from the IFPS [24]. Data from multiple years prior to the policy implementation is included to help account for any changes that may have occurred in England’s out-of-home food sector before the policy came into effect.

The study sample for IFPS was recruited from Nielsen Consumer Insights Global Panel and their partners’ panels. A random sample of participants aged 18–100 years were invited to complete the IFPS survey [25]. Online surveys were completed between November and December annually. Thus, the post-policy surveys were conducted 7–8 months after the menu labels were required to be displayed in England (by April 2022).

The conceptual framework used in this study assumes that for nutrition labelling to positively influence eating decisions, nutrition information must be noticed and then used in different ways to promote healthier eating. In addition to noticing and using nutrition information, outcomes included frequency of OOH eating and four other behaviours related to menu labelling at restaurants. Outcomes and potential confounders are defined in TableÌý1. Participants who visited a restaurant within the last 6 months (TableÌý1) were asked a series of questions about their behaviours at restaurants, and as such, analyses for all study outcomes were restricted to that population. The outcomes pertaining to noticing, use and impact of nutrition information related to in-person restaurant visits, while the eating out frequency outcome also included takeaway and other food obtained out-of-home. Sex was chosen as a potential confounder instead of gender identity due to small sample sizes in gender categories other than Male or Female. The IFPS was reviewed by and received ethics clearance through a University of Waterloo Research Ethics Committee (REB# 30829). A full description of IFPS methods can be found at .

Table 1 IFPS 2019–2022 survey questions, survey response options, and variable coding for analysis

Study design

The implementation of mandatory calorie labelling in England in 2022 enables examination of behavioural changes across years, comparing England as the intervention country to comparator countries without a policy. The countries within the International Food Policy Study (IFPS) have varying mandatory menu labelling policies, with some mandatory at the national levels, others at the state/province level and others with no mandatory menu labelling policy. Thus, the multi-country IFPS survey includes large populations that were and were not exposed to mandatory calorie (or other energy unit) labelling policies at the time of data collection.

This study compared pre-post changes in consumer behaviour in England in 2022 (post-policy) to the pre-policy years of 2019-21. Using a ‘natural experimental’ approach, changes in England from year to year were compared to jurisdictions with no policy for the entire study period. Within this natural experiment framework, England was designated as the intervention. IFPS jurisdictions with no policy throughout the entire study period were designated as the comparator group - that is: all of Mexico, jurisdictions of Australia and Canada without a policy, and jurisdictions of the United Kingdom without a policy (TableÌý2).

Table 2 Categorisation of jurisdictions according to presence or absence of mandatory menu labelling policies before 2019 data collection [9]

Modelling approach

All statistical analyses were conducted in Stata 17 [26]. Descriptive statistics were calculated to summarise sociodemographic characteristics of the study sample by year and for England and the comparator group separately. Binary outcomes were the six outcome variables in TableÌý1. Binary outcomes were modelled using survey-weighted mixed effects logistic regression, with clustering at the country level. Post-stratification sample weights were constructed using a raking algorithm with population estimates in each country separately based on age group, sex, region, and (except in Canada) ethnicity. Weights were subsequently rescaled to each sample size. Models were adjusted for potential confounders listed in TableÌý1, and included indicator variables for England vs. comparator group and study year. To estimate the potential differences in pre-post changes between England and the comparator, two-way interactions between policy group and study year were included. The marginal probability of each outcome was calculated by year and policy status [27]. Difference-in-differences were calculated for the changes from each consecutive year (2019 to 2020, 2020 to 2021, 2021 to 2022) in England compared to the changes in those years in the comparator. To explore the potential for spillover effects between England and the rest of the UK, we performed sensitivity analyses using the same outcomes, but we separated the rest of the UK countries (Scotland, Wales, and Northern Ireland) into a third group and described trends and outcomes.

A similar approach was used for the outcome frequency of eating food prepared away from home (online or in-person) in the last seven days using survey-weighted negative binomial regressions. Marginal means were calculated by year and policy status, and difference-in-differences were calculated for the changes from each consecutive year (2019 to 2020, 2020 to 2021, 2021 to 2022) in the policy group compared to the changes in those years in the comparator [27].

Results

A total of 67,960 adults completed the IFPS surveys in 2019–2022 across the four countries. A total of 46,809 people met the inclusion criteria described in TableÌý2 of being either from England or a jurisdiction without a comparable menu labelling policy. Among those, 40,209 (85.9%) participants reported visiting a restaurant within the last 6 months and answered the questions for the outcomes used for this analysis. Of this sample size that met all inclusion criteria, 467 observations (1.2%) were removed due to missing data on ethnicity, and a further 117 observations (0.3%) were removed due to missing data on education. The final sample included 39,625 respondents (2019 = 10,737; 2020 = 8,609; 2021 = 9,967; 2022 = 10,312).

TableÌý3 describes the sample characteristics, stratified by policy status and year. There was a greater proportion of high education participants in the comparator group compared to England. Participants in the comparator were slightly older than in England. Both groups reported a higher proportion of low income adequacy (i.e. not easy to make ends meet).

Table 3 Sample characteristics (data are unweighted N, weighted %; or weighted mean (SD)

Noticed nutrition information

There were no significant differences in noticing nutrition information between years in the comparator. In England, the probability of noticing nutrition information increased from 16.0% (95% CI 15.6 to 16.4) in 2020 to 19.7% (95% CI 19.1 to 20.2) in 2021 with a further increase to 25.8% (95% CI 25.5 to 26.1) in 2022 (Fig.Ìý1a). There was no evidence of a difference in changes between England and the comparator in 2020 vs. 2019. For 2021 vs. 2020, the difference in the probability of noticing was 2.9% points (95% CI 1.7 to 4.1) higher in England compared to the comparator. For 2022 vs. 2021, the difference was 4.8% points (95% CI 2.5 to 7.1) higher in England compared to the comparator (Fig.Ìý3).

Fig. 1
figure 1

a-b. Marginal probability of (a) noticing and (b) using nutrition information from 2019–2022 for England and the comparator estimated from mixed effects logistic regression model adjusted for age, sex, education, perceived income adequacy, and ethnicity. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals and are presented for both England and the comparator. The confidence intervals for England are narrow, at ± 0.6% or less for the outcomes

Used nutrition information

In England, the probability of using nutrition information increased from 8.0% (95% CI 7.5 to 8.4) in 2020 to 11.8% (95% CI 10.9 to 12.6) in 2021 and further increased to 13.5% (95% CI 13.1 to 13.9) in 2022 (Fig.Ìý1b). There was no evidence of a difference in changes between England and the comparator in 2020 vs. 2019. For 2021 vs. 2020, the difference in the probability of using nutrition information was 2.7% points (95% CI 2.0 to 3.4) higher in England compared to the comparator. For 2022 vs. 2021, the difference was smaller and not statistically significant (Fig.Ìý3).

Ordered something different

There were no significant differences in ordering something different because of nutrition information in restaurants between years in the comparator. There was a slight reduction in ordering something different in 2020 vs. 2019 in England, after which there were significant increases in England in 2021 and 2022 (Fig.Ìý2a). In England, the probability of ordering something different increased from 12.6% (95% CI 12.4 to 12.7) in 2020 to 15.2% (95% CI 14.7 to 15.6) in 2021 and a further increase to 17.7% (95% CI 17.6 to 17.8) in 2022 (Fig.Ìý2a). For 2022 vs. 2021, the difference in the probability of ordering something different was 2.8% points (95% CI 1.8 to 3.9) greater in England compared to the comparator (Fig.Ìý3).

Fig. 2
figure 2

a-d. Marginal probability (%) of (a) ordered something different, (b) ate less of order, (c) changed restaurants visited, and (d) ate at restaurants less often from 2019–2022 for England and the comparator. Estimations from mixed effects logistic regression model adjusted for age, sex, education, perceived income adequacy, and ethnicity. Error bars represent 95% confidence intervals and are presented for both England and the comparator. The confidence intervals for England are narrow, at ± 0.5% or less for the outcomes

Ate less of food ordered

There were no significant differences in eating less of the food ordered because of nutrition information between years in the comparator. In England, the probability of eating less of the food ordered increased from 12.3% (95% CI 12.1 to 12.5) in 2020 to 14.4% (95% CI 14.2 to 14.7) in 2021 and reduced to 12.8% (95% CI 12.6 to 13.0) in 2022 (Fig.Ìý2b). There was no evidence of a difference in changes between England and the comparator in 2020 vs. 2019. For 2021 vs. 2020, the difference in the probability of eating less of the food ordered was 2.3% points (95% CI 1.7 to 2.9) greater in England compared to the comparator (Fig.Ìý3). For 2022 vs. 2021, the difference in the probability of eating less of the food ordered was 1.6% points (95% CI 0.3 to 2.9) lower in England compared to the comparator.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Difference-in-differences (DiD) estimates for changes in each behavioural outcome in England versus comparator jurisdictions (2019–2022). Estimates are from mixed effects logistic regression model adjusted for age, sex, education, perceived income adequacy, and ethnicity. Positive values indicate greater increases in the outcome in England relative to the comparator group, whereas negative values indicate smaller increases (or larger decreases) in the outcome in England relative to the comparator group

Changed restaurants visited

There were no significant differences in changing restaurants visited because of nutrition information between years in the comparator. In England, the probability of changing restaurants visited increased from 8.3% (95% CI 8.2 to 8.4) in 2020 to 11.3% (95% CI 11.0 to 11.6) in 2021 and reduced to 9.0% (95% CI 8.8 to 9.2) in 2022 (Fig.Ìý2c). For 2020 vs. 2019, the change in the probability of changing restaurants visited was 0.5% points (95% CI 0.2 to 0.8) greater in England compared to the comparator (Fig.Ìý3). In 2021 vs. 2020, the difference in the probability of changing restaurants visited was 2.6% points (95% CI 1.4 to 3.8) greater in England compared to the comparator (Fig.Ìý3). In 2022 vs. 2021, the difference in the probability of changing restaurants visited was 3.8% points (95% CI 2.6 to 5.0) lower in England compared to the comparator.

Ate at restaurants less often

There were no significant differences in eating at restaurants less often because of nutrition information between years in the comparator. The probability of eating at restaurants less often was lower in England compared to the comparator in all years (Fig.Ìý2d). For 2020 vs. 2019, the difference in the probability of eating at restaurants less often was 2.9% points (95% CI 0.5 to 5.3) greater in England compared to the comparator (Fig.Ìý2). For 2021 vs. 2020, the difference in the probability of eating at restaurants less often was 3.1% points (95% CI 2.4 to 3.8) lower in England compared to the comparator, as England returned to baseline levels after 2020 (Figs.Ìý2d and 3). For 2022 vs. 2021, the difference in the probability of eating at restaurants less often was 1.2% points (95% CI 0.5 to 1.8) greater in England compared to the comparator (Fig.Ìý3).

Frequency of eating out

Frequency of eating out decreased from 2019 to 2020 in both England and the comparator (Fig.Ìý4a). For the difference-in-difference results, there was no significant differences between years comparing changes in England to the comparator. There was no evidence of a difference in changes between England and the comparator in 2020 vs. 2019. For 2021 vs. 2020, the change in frequency of eating out was 0.3 occasions per week (95% CI 0.2 to 0.3) higher in England compared to the comparator. For 2022 vs. 2021, there was no significant difference in changes between years (Fig.Ìý4b).

Fig. 4
figure 4

a-b. (a) Frequency of eating out of home from 2019–2022 for England and the comparator, (b) Difference-in-differences between years for frequency eating out of home

Sensitivity analysis

The sensitivity analysis found substantial evidence of spillover effects from England to the rest of the UK. Similar to England, the rates of noticing and using nutrition information in the rest of the UK increased from 2021 to 2022 (Supplementary Fig.Ìý1a, b). Rates of ordering something different in the rest of the UK also closely tracked those in England (Supplementary Fig.Ìý1c). Although rates of eating less of the food ordered were lower in the rest of the UK in 2020 and 2021, they increased to a similar level as England in 2022 (Supplementary Fig.Ìý1d). Rates of changed restaurant visited in the rest of the UK closely tracked those in England (Supplementary Fig.Ìý1e). Rates of eating at restaurants less often in the rest of the UK followed similar trends to England from 2019 to 2022 (Supplementary Fig.Ìý1f). Given the rest of the UK was included in the comparator group in the main analysis, these spillover effects likely diluted any policy effects identified in the main analysis.

Discussion

Statement of principal findings

We observed an increase in self-reported noticing and using nutrition information in England after the mandatory calorie labelling policy, and these increases were larger than comparator jurisdictions without a comparable policy. However, when examining how labelling was used, the only consistent change compared to comparator jurisdictions was an increase in ordering something different. We did not find evidence of participants eating OOH less often after policy implementation in England. Some behavioural changes occurred in England in 2021, which may have been due to restaurants implementing calorie labelling in preparation for the official policy implementation date in April 2022.

Strengths and limitations of the study

This is the first multi-country study to examine changes in behaviours associated with menu labelling after implementation in the OOH food sector, allowing comparisons to jurisdictions without policy implementation. This approach enhances the robustness of our analysis of national-level policies compared to relying on pre-post assessments without comparators. While the comparator group included varying contexts that may influence behaviours related to menu labelling, grouping jurisdictions without a labelling policy together helped mitigate any country-specific influences, enhancing generalisability. The large study population increases statistical power and the ability to observe small effect sizes that could nevertheless have public health impact. There was high internal consistency with the same questions asked across time and place [24]. The inclusion of multiple years prior to the policy implementation in England in 2022 (2019–2021), serves as a more robust baseline than a typical before-and-after study using single data points before and after. This extended timeframe, including pre-COVID data from 2019, offers additional context for interpreting changes potentially influenced by the COVID-19 pandemic in 2020 and 2021. Additionally, our study’s diversity of outcomes explores the association between implementation of the policy in England and mechanisms of policy effect on consumer behaviour, beginning with noticing menu labels followed by types of use.

Our study has limitations. The reliance on self-reported behaviours introduces bias inherent in surveys, but we assume this bias was consistent across survey years and countries, limiting the impact on our assessment of change. While trends in comparator jurisdictions remained relatively stable from 2019 to 2022, the specific impact of the intervention may be specific to England. We included the rest of the UK in the comparator group, but found some evidence of spillover effects from England to the rest of the UK. If anything, this would diminish our ability to detect a difference between England and the comparator. Similar spillover effects could potentially exist in Canada and Australia, where some jurisdictions have labelling laws and other areas do not. However, the existence of spillover would suggest that our estimates of changes in England are conservative. Causality remains inferential despite the controlled before-and-after study design, with the possibility of unaccounted-for co-interventions influencing our outcomes of interest. We saw some reductions in OOH eating frequency in 2020, likely due to Coronavirus disease (COVID-19) related business closures, but frequency returned to baseline levels in 2021. We expect that COVID-19 related impacts to OOH eating impacted intervention and comparator groups similarly. Although it is possible that COVID-19-related policies influenced OOH eating behaviours in England differently from the comparator group, we did not observe significant difference-in-differences effects for eating out frequency. This strengthens our confidence that the observed results for other behavioural outcomes were not primarily driven by differential changes in COVID-related lockdowns or out-of-home eating patterns.

There were slightly reduced sample sizes in 2020 and 2021, likely due to fewer respondents visiting restaurants within the past 6 months during the COVID-19 pandemic. While this may have reduced statistical power to detect small differences, the sample sizes remained sufficiently large for robust analyses. Furthermore, by adjusting for key confounders such as age, sex, education, income adequacy, and ethnicity, we aimed to account for potential time-varying differences in the types of participants who visited restaurants over time. We did not observe evidence of reduced precision in tests involving these years. The similar trends in eating out frequency strengthens the validity of the parallel trends assumption, although it is possible that unmeasured country-specific factors, such as variations in reopening rates or economic conditions, could introduce variability. Concerns regarding background trends in each policy group are somewhat mitigated by the inclusion of three baseline years. However, an interrupted time series analysis may have better accounted for pre-existing time trends, as our comparator jurisdictions may not capture all external factors influencing the study outcomes over time. The study’s analysis period covers 2019–2022, approximately seven to eight months post-policy implementation in England and future work could follow up further. The generalisability of this study to other countries could be limited if specific policies differ in a way that is related to consumer responses. For example, both the size and placement of calorie labels can modify the effects of menu labels on calories purchased [28].

Interpretation and implications of findings

The results of this study align with previous evidence that mandatory calorie labelling policies can increase noticing of nutrition information. In England, pre-post exit-surveys of 6,578 OOH customers found that 16.5% of participants reported noticing calorie labels pre-policy and 31.8% reported noticing calorie labels post-policy. However, the authors also found no evidence of change in the energy content of purchases pre-post policy implementation [29]. This consistency in prevalence of noticing nutrition information is particularly notable given the different types of data collected between the two studies. Although the current study found that noticing nutrition information increased after mandatory calorie labelling in England, there is still room for improvement as rates of noticing remained below 30%. Rates of noticing in the UK are less than those reported in some studies based in the United States, which could be due to labelling prominence. Previous research in England found that about two-thirds of businesses sampled in August–November 2022 (post implementation) had clear or legible calorie information, and only 15% followed all compliance criteria [30]. There may also be a diminution of effect on the pathway from noticing to using nutrition information. Recent work found only 22% of people in England who noticed nutrition information also reported using it [29]. Policymakers who are considering implementing mandatory menu labels may consider how to make labels more noticeable and how to enhance the effects of labels. For example, greater display size, increased use of colour, and consumer familiarity with labels are associated with greater attention to labels [31]. Customers eating OOH might also prioritise factors other than health, such as indulgence, financial considerations, or convenience [32]. Understanding consumer expectations and the mechanisms through which people interact with the OOH food environment is needed to inform policies that align with real-world behaviours. Greater public communication that increases motivation to change may also improve the effects of the policy [33].

We also found that although the mandatory calorie labelling policy was associated with greater noticing than the comparator, there were smaller effects on using calorie information and ordering something different, suggesting a diminishing effect along the potential chain of effect leading to changes in dietary behaviour. Previous work has also identified this diminution of effect between noticing and using nutrition information. A cross-sectional analysis of noticing and using calorie labels at a fast-food chain in the United States found that 60% of participants noticed calorie labels on menus, but only 16% reported using them [34].

There were some changes in 2020 that were potentially due to the influence of the COVID-19 pandemic on OOH eating behaviours. Ordering something different slightly decreased in 2020, but it increased each subsequent year in England. There was an increase in reporting eating at restaurants less often during 2020 in England, but responses returned to baseline rates in subsequent years. Eating out frequency decreased in 2020 in both England and the comparator group, but both returned to near 2019 levels in subsequent years.

Despite the absence of mandatory labelling policies elsewhere in the UK, sensitivity analyses revealed similar trends to those observed in England, suggesting possible spillover effects. These spillover effects may have occurred if it was more efficient for international companies that do business in the UK to implement the same menu changes across all UK countries.

Unanswered questions and future research

Future research may benefit from exploring more aspects of OOH eating in response to calorie labelling policies to more clearly understand how it affects all steps of the putative causal pathway to dietary change. It is also unknown whether there are differential behavioural responses to mandatory calorie labelling policies according to individual or eating occasion characteristics. For example, baseline nutrition knowledge and motivation to change may influence the impact of labelling policies and some eating occasions, such as dining out for special occasions, may be more resistant to change than others. Longer follow-up periods could provide a more thorough understanding of policy impact. There is growing evidence from grocery retail that interpretive labels result in more change in purchasing or greater ease of use than simple quantitative information, although the effects of interpretive labels may also depend on the specific type of label and the context [35,36,37,38]. Further work is required to understand the impact of interpretive nutrition labels (e.g. traffic light and warning labels) in the OOH sector. Finally, more work is needed to determine whether the increases in noticing nutrition information can translate into behaviour change by identifying and addressing barriers along the putative pathway of causation from labelling to behaviour change [39].

Conclusions

The introduction of a mandatory calorie labelling policy in England was associated with increases in noticing nutrition information, using nutrition information, and ordering something different and these changes were greater than in control jurisdictions without a policy. There was no evidence that the introduction of the policy was associated with changes in eating less of order, changing restaurant visited, or frequency of eating at restaurants. Further work is required to translate changes in noticing and using menu labels into health promoting behavioural changes.

Data availability

Data are available upon reasonable request to the International Food Policy Study team (see

References

  1. WHO. The out-of-home food environment: report of a WHO regional office for Europe and public health England expert meeting, 10 June 2021. Copenhagen; 2022.

  2. WHO. The out-of-home food sector – exponential growth in an unregulated market. World Health Organ [Internet]. 2021; Available from:

  3. Nago ES, Lachat CK, Dossa RAM, Kolsteren PW. Association of Out-of-Home eating with anthropometric changes: A systematic review of prospective studies. Crit Rev Food Sci Nutr. 2014;54(9):1103–16.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  4. Lachat C, Nago E, Verstraeten R, Roberfroid D, Van Camp J, Kolsteren P. Eating out of home and its association with dietary intake: A systematic review of the evidence. Obes Rev. 2012;13(4):329–46.

    Ìý CASÌý Ìý Ìý

  5. Gesteiro E, García-Carro A, Aparicio-Ugarriza R, González-Gross M. Eating out of home: influence on nutrition, health, and policies: A scoping review. Nutrients. 2022;14(6).

  6. Livingstone MBE, Pourshahidi LK. Portion size and obesity. Adv Nutr. 2014;5(6):829–34.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  7. Block JP, Condon SK, Kleinman K, Mullen J, Linakis S, Rifas-Shiman S et al. Consumers’ Estimation of calorie content at fast food restaurants: cross sectional observational study. BMJ Online. 2013;346(7912).

  8. Polden M, Jones A, Adams J, Bishop T, Burgoine T, Essman M, et al. Kilocalorie labelling in the out-of-home sector: an observational study of business practices and consumer behaviour prior to implementation of the mandatory calorie labelling policy in England, 2022. Ó£»¨ÊÓƵ. 2023;23(1):1088–1088.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  9. Rincón-Gallardo Patiño S, Zhou M, Gomes FDS, Lemaire R, Hedrick V, Serrano E et al. Effects of menu labeling policies on transnational restaurant chains to promote a healthy diet: A scoping review to inform policy and research. Nutrients. 2020;12(6).

  10. Legislation.gov.uk. The Calorie Labelling (Out of Home Sector) (England) Regulations 2021. UK Statut Instrum. 2021.

  11. GOV.UK. Calorie labelling in the out of home sector: implementation guidance. Dep Health Soc Care. 2021.

  12. Theis DRZ, Adams J. Differences in energy and nutritional content of menu items served by popular UK chain restaurants with versus without voluntary menu labelling: A cross-sectional study. PLoS ONE. 2019;14(10).

  13. Zlatevska N, Neumann N, Dubelaar C. Mandatory calorie disclosure: A comprehensive analysis of its effect on consumers and retailers. J Retail. 2018;94(1):89–101.

    Ìý Ìý

  14. Crockett RA, King SE, Marteau TM, Prevost AT, Bignardi G, Roberts NW et al. Nutritional labelling for healthier food or non-alcoholic drink purchasing and consumption. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2018;(2).

  15. Petimar J, Zhang F, Cleveland LP, Simon D, Gortmaker SL, Polacsek M et al. Estimating the effect of calorie menu labeling on calories purchased in a large restaurant franchise in the Southern united States: Quasi-experimental study. BMJ. 2019;367.

  16. Petimar J, Zhang F, Rimm EB, Simon D, Cleveland LP, Gortmaker SL et al. Changes in the calorie and nutrient content of purchased fast food meals after calorie menu labeling: A natural experiment. PLoS Med. 2021;18(7).

  17. Fernandes AC, Oliveira RC, Proenca RPC, Curioni CC, Rodrigues VM, Fiates GMR. Influence of menu labeling on food choices in real-life settings: A systematic review. Nutr Rev. 2016;74(8):534–48.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  18. Downs JS, Wisdom J, Wansink B, Loewenstein G. Supplementing menu labeling with calorie recommendations to test for facilitation effects. Am J Public Health. 2013;103(9):1604–9.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  19. Elbel B, Mijanovich T, Dixon LB, Abrams C, Weitzman B, Kersh R et al. In. Calorie labeling, fast food purchasing and restaurant visits. 2013. pp. 2172–9.

  20. Vadiveloo MK, Dixon LB, Elbel B. Consumer purchasing patterns in response to calorie labeling legislation in new York City. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2011;8.

  21. Vanderlee L, White CM, Hammond D. Evaluation of a voluntary nutritional information program versus calorie labelling on menus in Canadian restaurants: A quasi-experimental study design. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2019;16(1).

  22. Burton S, Kees J, Chairholder WM, Marketing I, Walton SM. Flies in the ointment?? Addressing potential impediments to Population-Based health benefits of restaurant menu labeling initiatives. J Public Policy Mark. 2012;31(2):1547–7207.

    Ìý Ìý

  23. Breck A, Mijanovich T, Weitzman BC, Elbel B. The current limits of calorie labeling and the potential for population health impact. J Public Policy Mark. 2017;36(2):227–35.

    Ìý Ìý

  24. Hammond D, Vanderlee L, White CM, Acton RB, White M, Roberto CA, et al. The conceptual framework for the international food policy study: evaluating the Population-Level impact of food policy. J Nutr. 2022;152(1):S1–12.

    Ìý Ìý

  25. Hammond D, White C, Rynard V. International Food Policy Study: Technical Report – 2022 Survey (Wave 6). 2023.

  26. StataCorp. Stata Statistical Software: Release 17. 2021.

  27. Williams R. Using the margins command to estimate and interpret adjusted predictions and marginal effects. Stata J. 2012;12:308–31.

    Ìý Ìý

  28. Robertson DA, Lunn PD. The effect of Spatial location of calorie information on choice, consumption and eye movements. Appetite. 2020;144.

  29. Polden M, Jones A, Essman M, Adams J, Bishop TRP, Burgoine T et al. Evaluating the association between the introduction of mandatory calorie labelling and energy consumed using observational data from the out-of-home food sector in England. Nat Hum Behav [Internet]. 2024 Nov 25 [cited 2024 Dec 23]; Available from:

  30. Polden M, Jones A, Essman M, Adams J, Bishop T, Burgoine T et al. Point-of-choice Kilocalorie labelling practices in large, out-of-home food businesses: a preobservational versus post observational study of labelling practices following implementation of the calorie labelling (Out of home Sector) (England) regulations 2021. BMJ Open. 2024;14(4).

  31. Bialkova S, van Trijp H. What determines consumer attention to nutrition labels? Food Qual Prefer. 2010;21(8):1042–51.

    Ìý Ìý

  32. Keeble M, Adams J, Burgoine T. Investigating experiences of frequent online food delivery service use: a qualitative study in UK adults. Ó£»¨ÊÓƵ. 2022;22(1).

  33. Grunert KG, Fernández-Celemín L, Wills JM, Bonsmann SSG, Nureeva L. Use and Understanding of nutrition information on food labels in six European countries. J Public Health. 2010;18(3):261–77.

    Ìý Ìý

  34. Green JE, Brown AG, Ohri-Vachaspati P. Sociodemographic disparities among Fast-Food restaurant customers who notice and use calorie menu labels. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2015;115(7):1093–101.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  35. Batista MF, de Carvalho-Ferreira JP, Thimoteo da Cunha D, De Rosso VV. Front-of-package nutrition labeling as a driver for healthier food choices: lessons learned and future perspectives. Compr Rev Food Sci Food Saf. 2023;22(1):535–86.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  36. Cecchini M, Warin L. Impact of food labelling systems on food choices and eating behaviours: A systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized studies. Obes Rev. 2016;17(3):201–10.

    Ìý CASÌý Ìý Ìý

  37. Mhurchu CN, Volkova E, Jiang Y, Eyles H, Michie J, Neal B, et al. Effects of interpretive nutrition labels on consumer food purchases: the starlight randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr. 2017;105(3):695–704.

    Ìý CASÌý Ìý

  38. Ducrot P, Julia C, Méjean C, Kesse-Guyot E, Touvier M, Fezeu LK, et al. Impact of different front-of-pack nutrition labels on consumer purchasing intentions: A randomized controlled trial. Am J Prev Med. 2016;50(5):627–36.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  39. Schruff-Lim EM, Van Loo EJ, van Kleef E, van Trijp HCM. Turning FOP nutrition labels into action: A systematic review of label + interventions. Food Policy. 2023;120.

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This independent research was commissioned and funded by the NIHR (NIHR200689, Policy Research Programme). The views expressed in this publication are those of the author(s) and not necessarily those of the NIHR or Department of Health and Social Care. This work was supported by the MRC Epidemiology Unit, University of Cambridge (grant number MC/UU/00006/7). Funding for the International Food Policy Study was provided by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) Project Grant (PJT-162167), with additional support for the adult survey from the National Institute of Diabetes and Digestive and Kidney Disorders of the National Institutes of Health (R01 DK128967). Funders had no role in study design; collection, analysis and interpretation of data; writing the report; and the decision to submit the report for publication.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

ME, TB, AJ, ER, SS, RS, MW, JA, and DH conceptualized the study. Methodology was developed by ME, TB, AJ, ER, SS, RS, MW, JA, and DH. Software programming and implementation were carried out by ME and SS. Formal analysis was conducted by ME. Resources for the study were provided by CMW, LV, and DH. Data curation was managed by CMW and DH. ME wrote the original draft of the manuscript. All authors (ME, TB, AJ, MP, ER, GS, SS, RS, LV, CMW, MW, DH, and JA) reviewed and edited the manuscript. Visualization was performed by ME. Supervision was provided by JA and MW. Project administration was managed by JA. Funding acquisition was secured by TB, AJ, ER, SS, RS, MW, JA, and DH. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael Essman.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study adhered to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained from all respondents prior to survey completion. The International Food Policy Study was reviewed by and received ethics clearance through a University of Waterloo Research Ethics Committee (REB # 30829).

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Electronic supplementary material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit .

About this article

Cite this article

Essman, M., Burgoine, T., Jones, A. et al. Assessing the impact of a mandatory calorie labelling policy in out-of-home food outlets in England on consumer behaviour: a natural experimental study. Ó£»¨ÊÓƵ 25, 955 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-025-22150-3

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-025-22150-3

Keywords