Ó£»¨ÊÓÆµ

Skip to main content
  • Research
  • Published:

Longitudinal associations between child, parenting, home and neighbourhood factors and children’s screen time through 4 to 7 years of age

Abstract

Background

Screen-based devices have become a pervasive feature in the lives of young children. Understanding the ecological influences determining young children’s screen use is imperative for promoting healthy screen habits. While individual studies have examined various factors influencing children’s screen use across the early years, few studies have explored multiple ecological influences simultaneously. This research examines longitudinal associations between children’s home environment, parent management practices and neighbourhood influences on children’s screen time, through 4 to 7 years of age.

Methods

Analyses draw on data from 2485 children in the Infant Cohort of a nationally representative sample of children and families participating in the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC). Bivariate and multiple linear regression models were developed to assess contributions of child, parenting, home and neighbourhood variables, measured at 4–5 years, in predicting children’s daily screen time at 6–7 years.

Results

Girls had lower screen time than boys at 6–7 years of age. Higher household socio-economic status, child participation in home learning activities and out-of-home extra-curricular activities, parenting consistency, and screen management practices including restricted bedroom access, presence and enforcement of screen rules, and parent-reported ease in managing screen time were also all significant predictors of lower daily screen time at age 6–7 years. Neighbourhood factors including liveability, sense of belonging and area-based socio-economic index were not significant predictors of children’s screen time, after child, parenting and home factors were included in the model.

Conclusions

Providing guidance to families about building healthy screen practices from an early age should focus on parenting consistency and management of screen time, as well as responsiveness to engage children in other activities. Home routines on screen use, when consistently managed, can provide young children with opportunities to learn to self-regulate their own screen use behaviours from an early age and build healthy screen time habits.

Peer Review reports

Background

Children’s access to screen-based devices has increased considerably over recent decades and the time spent on these devices is a prominent aspect of contemporary childhoods. In Australia, research has indicated that children are spending up to 30% of their waking time engaged in screen-based activities, such as watching TV and movies, and playing video games [1]. More than 50% of Australian parents have also reported that their child does not spend a day without screen-based technology [2]. Screen time is considered to displace the time available for other important developmental experiences, especially engaging in social interactions and outdoor activities, with many concerns focused on increases in children’s solitary and sedentary behaviours [3]. While screen-based activities can be designed for educational purposes and to stimulate imagination and creativity [4, 5], their pervasive use has raised concerns about the impacts of excessive screen time for children’s development, with studies showing negative impacts on physical, cognitive and psychosocial development [6,7,8]. Excessive screen use has been linked to increased sedentary behaviour [9], poorer sleep [7], lower executive function [10] and language skills [11], and externalising and internalising behaviour problems [12], underscoring the need to support balanced and developmentally appropriate digital engagement. Early childhood is a critical time in which health behaviours and home routines are established that can persist through childhood and adolescence [13,14,15]. Setting the foundations for healthy screen use during this early formative developmental period can provide children with opportunities to self-regulate their own screen time with important implications for longer-term development and wellbeing.

The bio-ecological systems model [16, 17] provides an important theoretical framework for understanding screen time behaviour in children by focusing on the social contexts in which children live. The model considers the multiple interconnected systems that surround the child and posits that a child’s behaviour is influenced by factors at the intrapersonal level as well as factors within children’s immediate micro-systems and their wider socio-cultural environment. Children’s early home environment is the primary social context in which young children engage with screen-based devices and parents have an important role as mediators of such activities [18]. The home environment is multifaceted and can be differentiated by relatively stable structural indicators such as family resources and household characteristics which create an environment for screen use behaviours to develop through availability and accessibility of devices, while more proximal factors such as parenting beliefs and practices influence children’s screen use through the provision and regulation of screen-based activities [19]. The broader community and neighbourhood environment in which a family lives can also influence children’s screen use by affording opportunities to support engagement in non-screen-based social interactions and physical activities outside of the home. In the Australian context, research has shown that parental perceptions of favourable neighbourhood features such as walkability and access to parks and a sense of community and neighbourhood social interactions are associated with increased outdoor play and reduced screen time among children [20, 21].

While a range of ecological factors have been found to influence children’s screen use in the early years [22,23,24,25], few studies have explored the multiple levels of influence at the intrapersonal, immediate micro-system and wider socio-cultural environment simultaneously to understand their relative contributions to variability in children’s daily screen time. Recent systematic reviews have consistently identified proximal influences, such as screen availability and parental rules about use as key determinants of young children’s screen time [25]. Other correlates include a greater breadth of family and socio-cultural demographics and neighbourhood characteristics [22, 23]. However, findings across studies have been inconsistent, suggesting that some influences depend on interactions with other individual, family and environmental factors, related to sample-specific characteristics. The aim of the current study is to examine the longitudinal associations of early child, parenting, home and neighbourhood influences at 4 and 5 years on children’s later screen time at 6–7 years of age, using a large population-level, representative sample of Australian children. With digital technology now a regular part of young children’s lives, identifying salient influences on screen time can be used to inform evidence-based public health initiatives to support families in creating healthy screen time habits during an important developmental period for young children.

Method

Study design

These analyses draw on data from the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC), a nationally representative prospective cohort study of children’s development from early childhood to young adulthood. LSAC’s study design and sampling frame has been previously described [26]. Data collection commenced in 2004 and the study used a two-stage cluster sampling design to recruit two cohorts of Australian children– an Infant cohort, aged 6–18 months (n = 5107) and a Kindergarten cohort, aged 4–5 years (n = 4983). The primary sampling unit is Australian postcodes, stratified by state and location (urban and rural split) of residence. Children were then randomly selected using the Australian Medicare universal healthcare database held by the Health Insurance Commission, in which > 90% of infants and 98% of 4-year-old children are registered. The recruited sample in each of the two cohorts were broadly representative of the Australian population, with the exception of children living in remote areas [27].

Participating families are followed in biennial waves, with data collected through home-based interviews with the primary caregiver, as well as through self-complete questionnaires completed by primary caregivers and teachers, and through direct child assessments. Mail-out questionnaires were also completed by the primary caregiver at a mid-point between the biennial data collections of Waves 3 and 4. The current analyses use data from Wave 3 (4–5 years), Wave 3.5 (5–6 years), and Wave 4 (6–7 years), collected in years 2008, 2009 and 2010, from the Infant Cohort of LSAC.

Measures

Child screen time

Children’s screen time was measured at Wave 4 during the primary caregiver home interview, when children were aged 6–7 years. Six items asked caregivers to report the total number of hours that their child spent: (1) watching television, DVDs/videos (television viewing); (2) using a computer, and (3) playing electronic games, separately for a typical weekday and weekend day. Average daily scores were calculated by summing hours reported for an average weekday (multiplied by five) and average weekend day (multiplied by two) and then dividing by seven to obtain a daily average for each form of screen exposure. Average daily television viewing time, computer use and time playing electronic games were summed to create a total score for daily screen time for each child.

Predictors of screen time

Predictors of children’s screen time were selected a-priori based on previous research [22, 23, 25] and are detailed in TableÌý1. These predictor variables were measured at Wave 3 and Wave 3.5 from the primary caregiver home interview and mail-out questionnaire. Drawing upon an ecological framework that focuses on the interplay of intrapersonal, micro-system and broader socio-cultural factors in determining children’s behaviour and development [16, 17], potential predictor variables were classified into child, parenting, home and neighbourhood factors.

Table 1 Potential predictors of children’s screen time

Data analytic approach

Sample selection and missing data

A total of 2485 primary caregivers completed the Wave 3, 3.5 and 4 home interview and questionnaire which collected information on the variables of interest. Participants in the analytic sample were more likely to have a higher socio-economic position, F(1,5092) = 391.485, p <.001; and were less likely to be Indigenous, χ2(1, 5107) = 84.06, p <.001, or have a language other than English at home, χ2(1, 5104) = 77.335, p <.001, compared to the original sample recruited for Wave 1 data collection.

Statistical Analysis

All variables listed in TableÌý1 were considered as potential predictors. Descriptive statistics were reported as means and standard deviations for continuous variables and frequencies for dichotomous variables. Bivariate correlations were used to explore associations between continuous variables used in the analyses. Linear regression was conducted to explore potential relationships between the child, parenting, home and neighbourhood factors and children’s total daily screen time as the outcome measure. All potential predictors that reached significance in the bivariate regression models (at p <.05) were included in a multiple linear regression. All relevant predictor variables were entered simultaneously, allowing for an evaluation of each predictor’s unique contribution to explaining children’s total daily screen time. Analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics (Version 29, IBM, Armonk, NY), and p-values were two-tailed, with p <.05 considered statistically significant. Regarding model assumptions, the histogram of the residuals appeared approximately bell-shaped, and the normal P-P plot showed a close fit to the diagonal line, suggesting that the residuals were approximately normally distributed. The scatterplot of standardised residuals showed that the data met the assumptions of homogeneity of variance and linearity. Multicollinearity was not a concern, with all VIFs below 2.

Results

Descriptive statistics for the variables used in the analyses are presented in TableÌý2. Children’s mean total screen time at 6–7 years of age was 2.57Ìýh (SD = 1.41) per day. Television viewing made up most of children’s screen time, with a mean of 1.77Ìýh (SD = 0.99) per day. Children spent a similar amount of time using a computer and playing electronic games with a mean of 0.42Ìýh (SD = 0.48) and 0.39Ìýh (SD = 0.49) per day, respectively. Bivariate correlations between continuous variables are presented in TableÌý3. Correlations were in the expected directions and almost all were significant (p &±ô³Ù;.05).

Table 2 Sample characteristics and descriptive statistics
Table 3 Bivariate correlations between continuous variables of interest

Predictors of screen time

TableÌý4 presents the results of the bivariate linear regression analyses. There were no significant associations between young children’s daily screen time at 6–7 years and the following variables: child age, Indigenous background, number of siblings in the home, adult involvement when using screens and neighbourhood liveability. These variables were not included in the final multiple linear regression model.

Table 4 Bivariate regression model for the predictors of children’s screen time at 6–7 years of age

TableÌý5 presents the results of the multiple linear regression analysis. The multiple linear regression model was significant, F(16, 2484) = 26.121, p <.001; adjusted R2 = 0.139. In this analysis, significant predictors of children’s daily screen time included child sex, child participation in home learning activities and out-of-home extra-curricular activities, parenting consistency, household socio-economic position, and screen management practices including bedroom access, screen use rules, enforcement of screen use rules and parent-reported ease of management of screen use.

Table 5 Multiple variable regression model for the predictors of children’s screen time at 6–7 years of age

Girls were more likely to have lower total screen time than boys (β = -0.09, p &±ô³Ù;.001). More frequent participation in home learning activities and out-of-home extra-curricular activities were both associated with lower screen time (β = -0.075, p <.001 and β = -0.063, p =.001, respectively). Higher parenting consistency was associated with lower screen time (β = -0.059, p =.005). A higher household socio-economic position was also associated with lower screen time (β = -0.152, p &±ô³Ù;.001). Access to more screen-based devices in the bedroom was associated with higher total screen time (β = 0.118, p <.001), while having more rules about screen use (β = -0.058, p <.05) and more frequent enforcement of screen use rules (β = -0.077, p <.001) were associated with lower total daily screen time. Parent-reported ease with child screen use management was associated with lower total daily screen use (β = -0.09, p &±ô³Ù;.001).

There were no significant associations between home language, child attention and emotion regulation, number of parents in the household, primary caregiver mental health, neighbourhood sense of belonging and the socio-economic index for the area (SEIFA) with children’s daily screen time (all p >.05), after controlling for all other predictor variables.

Discussion

The current study explored child, parenting, home and neighbourhood predictors of young children’s screen time in a large national sample of Australian families. At the time of data collection (2010), 6–7 year-old children had an average of 2.6Ìýh per day of screen time. Girls were found to have lower screen time than boys. Children in families who had higher socio-economic status also had lower daily levels of screen use, with SES emerging as the most significant predictor in the model. Investigation of home and parenting management practices related to screen time identified that lower screen time was associated with a more consistent parenting style (e.g., following through on instructions given to child and ensuring compliance), higher engagement with children in home learning activities and higher child participation in out-of-home activities. Parents of children with lower levels of screen time also reported having more rules about screen time, consistent enforcement of screen time rules, and greater ease in managing screen time, which were some of the strongest predictors of lower screen use. In examining longitudinal associations for a comprehensive set of ecological factors simultaneously on children’s screen time, the findings from the current study have important implications for public health initiatives and intervention efforts that aim to support healthy screen use habits in young children. While the overall model explained a modest amount of variance in children’s screen time, the findings identify several key modifiable factors that can inform targeted intervention efforts.

While data in the current study were collected in 2010, the average level of daily screen time usage for Australian children at this time is in line with recent international findings. Data collected in early 2020 from a nationally representative survey of parents and children in the United States [28] reported that average daily screen time for children aged 8 years and younger was 2Ìýh 24Ìýmin and this usage was also primarily television and video viewing. The researchers also noted that this amount of screen time usage had remained relatively stable, for this age group, over the nine years in which this survey in the United States had been conducted. However, it is important to acknowledge that the proliferation of smartphones and mobile devices, as well as the global COVID-19 pandemic, has likely altered screen time dynamics, especially in relation to hand-held devices and increases in excessive screen time concerns among children [29, 30]. The findings from the current study provide a foundational understanding of screen time behaviours that can be used to compare with more recent data to explore shifts in usage patterns, parenting challenges and child development in response to evolving digital environments.

In the current study, the finding that boys had higher overall screen time than girls may reflect the type of screen time measured in the current study that included TV viewing, electronic game playing and computer use. Previous systematic reviews of correlates of screen use during early childhood [22, 23, 25] have consistently reported no association with child sex, although most studies included in these reviews measured traditional TV viewing only. Studies that distinguish between different types of screen-based activities have found that, while television viewing is similar across sex, boys tend to spend more time playing video games than girls and that these differences increase with age [31, 32].

In the bivariate analyses, poorer attention and emotion regulation were associated with higher screen time; however, this association did not remain significant in the adjusted model. This may be related to the developmental stage of the sample and the role of adults in enabling access to screen-based devices for this age group. Research with young children (aged 5 and under) has found that parental monitoring and limit setting are associated with children’s screen time [33]. As children grow older, individual self-regulatory ability may play a more influential role in predicting screen time, as supervision decreases and access to devices increases. This underscores the importance of further detailed longitudinal analyses in this area to examine if children’s increasing capacities for self-regulation affect the level of parental involvement and how parents act to monitor screen time across developmental stages.

Parental engagement with their child in home learning activities, as well as child participation in out-of-home extra-curricular activities were associated with lower screen time, aligning with existing research that emphasises the role of non-screen-based activities in promoting healthier screen time behaviours. For example, increased access to cognitively stimulating activities in the home, such as shared reading and educational play, and interventions that promotion parent-child interaction through these activities have both been associated with reduced screen use among young children and infants [34, 35] Such interactional opportunities with others, both inside and outside of the home, not only displaces screen time but also provide children with opportunities for physical movement and social engagement.

The findings also identified a significant association between family socio-economic status (SES) and screen time, similar to previous research [28]. SES represents the social, cultural and economic capital available in families and is an important factor that affects the resources parents can invest in their children [36]. Higher SES enables families greater access to alternative non-screen-based resources and activities. As a consequence, children from higher SES backgrounds are more likely to have lower reliance on screen-based entertainment. Notably, SES had one of the strongest effects in the model, reinforcing its central role in shaping children’s screen behaviours and the need for intervention efforts to consider broader socio-economic factors.

Consistent with previous research [25, 37], the current study found parental screen management practices related to restricted bedroom access, and the presence and enforcement of screen use rules were associated with lower time spent in screen-based activities. Crucially, one of the strongest predictors of lower screen time was parent-reported ease in managing their child’s screen use, suggesting that parental self-efficacy in managing screen habits plays an important role. Providing guidance to families around healthy screen habits in the home as well as increasing parental confidence in managing their child’s screen use may be potential strategies for future interventions. Broader parenting behaviours were also significantly associated with children’s screen time in the adjusted models, supporting the importance of consistent routines and practices in the home in facilitating the development of healthy habits in children [38,39,40].

Measures focused on the neighbourhood environment of families including the area-based socio-economic index (SEIFA), as well as parental perceptions of belonging and liveability of the wider neighbourhood environment were not associated with children’s screen time in the adjusted models, suggesting that more proximal factors within the family microsystem and home environment may likely offer the most effective targets for intervention to reduce screen time of young children, and may have more relevance and impact on older children and adolescents. The measures used in the current study, however, did not examine aspects of neighbourhood safety and crime, which may influence parental decisions regarding the provision of screen-based activities inside the home compared to outdoor play [20].

Strengths and limitations

The current study has several strengths including the use of a large cohort, the inclusion of multiple independent predictor variables, and the exploration of longitudinal relations between these predictor variables and children’s screen time. There are limitations, however, that should be considered when interpreting the findings. Children’s screen time was measured using subjective parent-report, which have been shown to both underestimate and overestimate exposure because of recall bias, social desirability bias or simply not being aware of screen viewing behaviours [41, 42]. The participants in this study were recruited in 2004, with data collection about children’s screen time at 6–7 years of age occurring in 2010. Since then, there has been significant change in the availability and type of screen-based devices and this means we have not captured time spent on more contemporary devices, such as smartphones and tablets. It is also important to note that quantity of screen time represents one aspect of children’s screen use behaviours. Beyond quantity, examining the contextual factors that influence screen time, such as the content (i.e., educational, entertainment), level of engagement (i.e., passive, active), and co-use (i.e., in the presence of peers or parents) will be important for future investigations. It is also possible that the association between the independent predictor variables and children’s screen time may be over-estimated due to common method variance with data across these variables collected from the same informant. Finally, although we used data from a well-defined birth cohort enabling the results to be generalised to a national population, participants from lower socio-economic groups and culturally diverse backgrounds were less likely to have complete data on the variables used in this study. Thus, the results may not generalise to these populations.

Conclusion

The current study explored child, parenting, home and neighbourhood predictors of young children’s screen time in a large national sample of Australian families. The findings suggest that supporting parents to implement screen management practices in the home, including limiting children’s access to screen-based devices in bedrooms, and having and enforcing rules about children’s screen use, may be potential targets to foster healthy screen time habits in young children. Support in these areas may contribute to increasing parent’s perceived ease in managing children’s screen use, which was also associated with children’s screen time. Additionally, strategies to reduce children’s screen time should include understandings of the importance of engaging children in alternative non-screen-based activities in the home that foster children’s learning in areas in which they have strong interests, as well as for engagement in out-of-home extra-curricular activities and community events. Given the study’s large, nationally representative sample, these findings can inform public health initiatives aimed at promoting balanced screen use in the early years through family-focused interventions that support parents in managing both screen and non-screen-based engagement with their children. Moreover, these results, which focus on screen use via televisions, computers and gaming consoles, provide a foundation for future research to examine the evolving digital landscape, including the growing use of mobile devices and how factors such as parenting practices and home environments continue to shape children’s screen use in the digital age. Later waves of data collection in the LSAC dataset offer opportunities to explore how early screen use influence subsequent digital engagement and development outcomes through to adolescence.

Data availability

The dataset analysed for the current study is available from the Australian Data Archive at https://dataverse.ada.edu.au/dataverse/lsac.

References

  1. Yu M, Baxter J. Australian children’s screen time and participation in extracurricular activities. The longitudinal study of Australian children annual statistical report 2015. Melbourne, Australia: Australian Institute of Family Studies; 2016.

    Ìý

  2. Gonski Institute for Education. Growing up digital Australia: Phase 1 technical report. In. Sydney, NSW: Gonski Institute for Education, UNSW; 2020.

  3. Hassinger-Das B, Brennan S, Dore RA, Golinkoff RM, Hirsh-Pasek K. Children and screens. Annu Rev Dev Psychol. 2020;2:69–92.

    Ìý Ìý

  4. Herodotou C. Young children and tablets: A systematic review of effects on learning and development. J Comput Assist Learn. 2018;34(1):1–9.

    Ìý Ìý

  5. Xiong Z, Liu Q, Huang X. The influence of digital educational games on preschool children’s creative thinking. Comput Educ. 2022;189:104578.

    Ìý Ìý

  6. Kerai S, Almas A, Guhn M, Forer B, Oberle E. Screen time and developmental health: results from an early childhood study in Canada. Ó£»¨ÊÓÆµ. 2022;22(1):310.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  7. Li C, Cheng G, Sha T, Cheng W, Yan Y. The relationships between screen use and health indicators among infants, toddlers, and preschoolers: A meta-analysis and systematic review. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(19):7324.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  8. Swider-Cios E, Vermeij A, Sitskoorn MM. Young children and screen-based media: the impact on cognitive and socioemotional development and the importance of parental mediation. Cogn Dev. 2023;66:101319.

    Ìý Ìý

  9. Venetsanou F, Antonis K, Vassilios G, Yannakoulia M. Physical activity in pre-school children: trends over time and associations with body mass index and screen time. Ann Hum Biol. 2019;46(5):393–9.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  10. Fitzpatrick C, Florit E, Lemieux A, Garon-Carrier G, Mason L. Associations between preschooler screen time trajectories and executive function. Acad Pediatr. 2025;25(2):102603.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  11. Madigan S, McArthur BA, Anhorn C, Eirich R, Christakis DA. Associations between screen use and child Language skills: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(7):665–75.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  12. Eirich R, McArthur BA, Anhorn C, McGuinness C, Christakis DA, Madigan S. Association of screen time with internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in children 12 years or younger: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2022;79(5):393–405.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  13. Guimarães RF, Mathieu ME, Reid RER, Henderson M, Barnett TA. Physical activity, screen time, and sleep trajectories from childhood to adolescence: the influence of sex and body weight status. J Phys Act Health. 2021;18(7):767–73.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  14. McArthur BA, Browne D, Tough S, Madigan S. Trajectories of screen use during early childhood: predictors and associated behavior and learning outcomes. Comput Hum Behav. 2020;113:106501.

    Ìý Ìý

  15. Trinh M-H, Sundaram R, Robinson SL, Lin T-C, Bell EM, Ghassabian A, Yeung EH. Association of trajectory and covariates of children’s screen media time. JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(1):71–8.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  16. Bronfenbrenner U, Evans GW. Developmental science in the 21st century: emerging questions, theoretical models, research designs and empirical findings. Soc Dev. 2000;9(1):115–25.

    Ìý Ìý

  17. Bronfenbrenner U, Morris PA. The Bioecological Model of Human Development. In: Handbook of Child Psychology. edn.

  18. Kluczniok K, Lehrl S, Kuger S, Rossbach H-G. Quality of the home learning environment during preschool age: domains and contextual conditions. Eur Early Child Educ Res J. 2013;21(3):420–38.

    Ìý

  19. Domoff SE, Borgen AL, Radesky JS. Interactional theory of childhood problematic media use. Hum Behav Emerg Technol. 2020;2(4):343–53.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  20. Baldwin J, Arundell L, Hnatiuk JA. Associations between the neighbourhood social environment and preschool children’s physical activity and screen time. Ó£»¨ÊÓÆµ. 2022;22(1):1065.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  21. Hunter S, Carson V, Timperio A, Salmon J, Carver A, Veitch J. Moderators of parents’ perceptions of the neighborhood environment and children’s physical activity, time outside, and screen time. J Phys Act Health. 2020;17(5):557–65.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  22. Duch H, Fisher EM, Ensari I, Harrington A. Screen time use in children under 3 years old: A systematic review of correlates. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2013;10(1):102.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  23. Hoyos Cillero I, Jago R. Systematic review of correlates of screen-viewing among young children. Prev Med. 2010;51(1):3–10.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  24. Paudel S, Jancey J, Subedi N, Leavy J. Correlates of mobile screen media use among children aged 0–8: A systematic review. BMJ Open. 2017;7(10):e014585.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  25. Veldman SLC, Altenburg TM, Chinapaw MJM, Gubbels JS. Correlates of screen time in the early years (0–5 years): A systematic review. Prev Med Rep. 2023;33:102214.

    Ìý CASÌý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  26. Soloff C, Lawrence D, Johnston R. Longitudinal study of Australian children technical paper no. 1: sample design. In. Melbourne, Australia: Australian Institute of Family Studies; 2005.

    Ìý

  27. Soloff C, Lawrence D, Misson S, Johnstone R. Longitudinal study of Australian children technical paper no. 3: wave 1 weighting and non-response. In. Melbourne, Australia: Australian Institute of Family Studies; 2006.

    Ìý

  28. Rideout V, Robb MB. The Common Sense census: Media use by kids age zero to eight, 2020. In. San Francisco, CA: Common Sense Media; 2020.

  29. Choi EJ, King GKC, Duerden EG. Screen time in children and youth during the pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Global Pediatr. 2023;6:100080.

    Ìý Ìý

  30. Trott M, Driscoll R, Iraldo E, Pardhan S. Changes and correlates of screen time in adults and children during the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. eClinicalMedicine 2022, 48.

  31. Jago R, Thompson JL, Sebire SJ, Wood L, Pool L, Zahra J, Lawlor DA. Cross-sectional associations between the screen-time of parents and young children: differences by parent and child gender and day of the week. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2014;11(1):54.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  32. Paulus FW, Joas J, Friedmann A, Fuschlberger T, Möhler E, Mall V. Familial context influences media usage in 0- to 4-year old children. Front Public Health 2024, 11.

  33. Tang L, Darlington G, Ma DWL, Haines J, on behalf of the Guelph Family Health S. Mothers’ and fathers’ media parenting practices associated with young children’s screen-time: a cross-sectional study. Ó£»¨ÊÓÆµ Obes. 2018;5(1):37.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  34. Detnakarintra K, Trairatvorakul P, Pruksananonda C, Chonchaiya W. Positive mother-child interactions and parenting styles were associated with lower screen time in early childhood. Acta Paediatr. 2020;109(4):817–26.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  35. Mendelsohn AL, Dreyer BP, Brockmeyer CA, Berkule-Silberman SB, Huberman HS, Tomopoulos S. Randomized controlled trial of primary care pediatric parenting programs: effect on reduced media exposure in infants, mediated through enhanced parent-child interaction. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2011;165(1):42–8.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  36. Conger RD, Donnellan MB. An interactionist perspective on the socioeconomic context of human development. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:175–99.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  37. Pyper E, Harrington D, Manson H. The impact of different types of parental support behaviours on child physical activity, healthy eating, and screen time: a cross-sectional study. Ó£»¨ÊÓÆµ. 2016;16(1):568.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  38. Sciberras E, Song JC, Mulraney M, Schuster T, Hiscock H. Sleep problems in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: associations with parenting style and sleep hygiene. Euro Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;26(9):1129–39.

    Ìý Ìý

  39. Leuba AL, Meyer AH, Kakebeeke TH, Stülb K, Arhab A, Zysset AE, Leeger-Aschmann CS, Schmutz EA, Kriemler S, Jenni OG, et al. The relationship of parenting style and eating behavior in preschool children. Ó£»¨ÊÓÆµ Psychol. 2022;10(1):275.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  40. Jansen PW, Giallo R, Westrupp EM, Wake M, Nicholson JM. Bidirectional associations between mothers’ and fathers’ parenting consistency and child BMI. Pediatrics. 2013;132(6):e1513–20.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý

  41. Robinson JL, Winiewicz DD, Fuerch JH, Roemmich JN, Epstein LH. Relationship between parental estimate and an objective measure of child television watching. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2006;3(1):43.

    Ìý Ìý Ìý Ìý

  42. Radesky JS, Weeks HM, Ball R, Schaller A, Yeo S, Durnez J, Tamayo-Rios M, Epstein M, Kirkorian H, Coyne S et al. Young children’s use of smartphones and tablets. Pediatrics 2020, 146(1).

  43. Sanson AV, Smart DF, Prior M, Oberklaid F, Pedlow R. The structure of temperament from age 3 to 7 years: age, sex, and sociodemographic influences. Merrill-Palmer Q. 1994;40(2):233–52.

    Ìý

  44. Canada S. National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth (NLSCY) Cycle 3 survey instruments: Parent questionnaire. In. Ottowa, Canada; 2000.

  45. Furukawa TA, Kessler RC, Slade T, Andrews G. The performance of the K6 and K10 screening scales for psychological distress in the Australian National survey of mental health and Well-Being. Psychol Med. 2003;33(2):357–62.

    Ìý CASÌý Ìý Ìý

  46. Blakemore T, Gibbings J, Strazdins L. Measurement of the socio-economic position of families. Aust Soc Policy J. 2009;8:121–69.

    Ìý

  47. Stone W, Hughes J. Social capital: empirical meaning and measurement validity (Research paper no. 27). In. Melbourne, Australia: Australian Institute of Family Studies; 2002.

    Ìý

  48. Australian Bureau of Statistics. Socio-economic indexes for areas. In. Canberra, Australia: Australian Bureau of Statistics; 2018.

Acknowledgements

This paper uses unit record data from Growing Up in Australia: The Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC). The LSAC study is conducted in current partnership between the Department of Social Services (DSS) and the Australian Institute of Family Studies (AIFS). The findings and views reported in this paper are those of the authors and should not be attributed to the DSS, or the AIFS. This research was supported by the Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence for the Digital Child (Project Number: CE200100022).

Funding

This research was supported by the Australian Research Council Centre of Excellence for the Digital Child (Project Number: CE200100022).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors contributed to the conceptualisation of the study. NH conducted the data analysis and drafted the manuscript. SW, DB, JB and DC critically reviewed the manuscript and assisted with final editing and writing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nicole Hayes.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Each wave of LSAC was approved by the Australian Institute of Family Study Ethics Committee and caregivers provided written informed consent. LSAC is conducted in accordance with the National Statement on Ethical Conduct in Research Involving Humans. Approval to access the LSAC dataset was obtained from the Department of Social Services and the Australian Data Archive.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit .

About this article

Cite this article

Hayes, N., White, S.L.J., Berthelsen, D. et al. Longitudinal associations between child, parenting, home and neighbourhood factors and children’s screen time through 4 to 7 years of age. Ó£»¨ÊÓÆµ 25, 1623 (2025). https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-025-22866-2

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-025-22866-2

Keywords